Sexual Anxiety Scale

Sexual Anxiety Scale‌‌‌‌‌‌‌

ERIN E. FALLIS,1 CHRISTINA GORDON, AND CHRISTINE PURDON, University of Waterloo

The Sexual Anxiety Scale (SAS) was developed to assess individuals’ affective response to sexual cues, or erotophobia/philia. The term erotophobia/philia (EE) refers to the tendency to respond to sexual stimuli with either negative or positive affect (Fisher, Byrne, White, & Kelley, 1988), and the primary measure of EE to date has been the Sexual Opinion Survey (SOS; Fisher et al., 1988). Although exhibiting good psychometric properties, the SOS focuses primarily on responses to homosexuality, media with sexual content, and a small range of sexual behaviours. The SAS was developed to assess affective response to a broader range of sexual cues in both the public and the private domain.

Sexual Anxiety Scale

The Sexual Anxiety Scale (SAS) is a self-report questionnaire that measures sexual anxiety in individuals. The SAS was developed by Fisher, Byrne, White, and Kelley (1988) to assess individuals’ affective response to sexual cues, or erotophobia/philia.

What is sexual anxiety?

Sexual anxiety is a fear or apprehension about sexual activity. It can be caused by a variety of factors, including negative experiences, cultural beliefs, and medical conditions. Sexual anxiety can have a significant impact on sexual function and quality of life.

What is the SAS?

The SAS is a 25-item self-report questionnaire that measures sexual anxiety. The items on the SAS are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) in terms of how much the statement applies to the individual.

How is the SAS scored?

The SAS is scored by summing the item scores. The total score can range from 25 to 125, with higher scores indicating a greater level of sexual anxiety.

What is the reliability and validity of the SAS?

The SAS has been shown to be a reliable and valid measure of sexual anxiety. The SAS has good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and discriminant validity.

How is the SAS used?

The SAS can be used for a variety of purposes, including:

  • Diagnosis: The SAS can be used to help diagnose sexual anxiety.
  • Assessment: The SAS can be used to assess the severity of sexual anxiety.
  • Treatment planning: The SAS can be used to help develop a treatment plan for sexual anxiety.
  • Monitoring progress: The SAS can be used to monitor progress in treatment for sexual anxiety.

What are the limitations of the SAS?

The SAS has a few limitations, including:

  • It is not a diagnostic tool. The SAS should not be used to diagnose sexual anxiety.
  • It is not a comprehensive measure of sexual anxiety. The SAS only measures a limited number of aspects of sexual anxiety.
  • It is not a sensitive measure of change. The SAS may not be sensitive to small changes in the level of sexual anxiety.

Conclusion

The SAS is a useful tool for assessing sexual anxiety in individuals. The SAS has been shown to be a reliable and valid measure of sexual anxiety, and it has been used in a variety of research studies and clinical settings.

Description

Respondents rate their degree of discomfort with a list of sexually relevant situations or stimuli on 11-point Likert-type scales ranging from Extremely Pleasurable to Extremely Discomforting. Items reflecting categories of sexual cues were written by members of our laboratory team and reviewed by two experts on sexuality who were not members of the team. The 56-item version of the scale was thus produced. The SAS was administered to a sample of undergraduate students at a midsized university in Ontario, Canada as part of a large test battery (N = 701). Reliability and validity were examined using a subset of the undergraduate students (n = 376) and a community sample of adults (n = 188). The demographic characteristics of the validation samples are shown in Table 1.‌‌

A factor analysis on responses from the undergraduate and community samples (N = 889) was conducted. This yielded a three-factor solution accounting for 49.5% of the variance. The first factor, Solitary and Impersonal Sexual Expression, accounted for 35.8% of the variance in the SAS and consists of 23 items pertaining to pornographic and erotic material, masturbation, and impersonal sexual experiences. The second factor, Exposure to Information, accounted for 8.1% of the variance in the SAS and consists of 14 items about giving or receiving information of a sexual nature. The third factor, Sexual Communication, accounted for 5.6% of the variance and includes 16 items reflecting openness to consensual sexual activity and communicating sexual likes and dislikes. Subscales based on these factors were then calculated and labeled accordingly.2‌ *p < .01 Correlation coefficients between the three factors were calculated for both samples, and these relationships may be seen in Table 2. Means and standard deviations for each factor for men and women in both samples are shown in Table 3. Response Mode and Timing The SAS is a self-report measure that takes between 5 and 15 minutes to complete. Scoring The SAS total score is calculated by summing the responses to each item. Higher scores indicate greater erotophobia. Individual scale scores can be calculated by summing the items in the relevant scale (see the Exhibit for the scale item key). 1Address correspondence to Erin Fallis, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G1, Canada; e-mail: [email protected] 2In these studies, respondents also rated the extent to which the sexual cue was likely to be avoided or approached so that behavior/attitude discrepancies could be explored. However, the scores on the two sets of ratings were redundant, with correlations > .92 in all samples. As such, it was decided that the approach/avoidance ratings were not a useful addition to the measure and have been dropped from the final version.

correlated with greater sexual satisfaction (Global Measure of Sexual Satisfaction; Lawrance & Byers, 1995), less antigay prejudice (Heterosexual Attitudes Toward Homosexuality Scale; Larsen, 1998), better sexual functioning (Sexual Functioning Questionnaire; Lawrance

TABLE 3

Means and Standard Deviations of Total SAS Scores correlated with greater sexual satisfaction (Global Measure of Sexual Satisfaction; Lawrance & Byers, 1995), less antigay prejudice (Heterosexual Attitudes Toward Homosexuality Scale; Larsen, 1998), better sexual functioning (Sexual Functioning Questionnaire; Lawrance

Total

2339.4

765.9

2775.3

858.4

2563.3

842.4

Factor 1

909.6

336.4

1318.2

447.6

1119.4

457.8

Factor 2

793.3

204.2

761.4

197.5

776.9

201.1

Factor 3

476.2

274.5

518.4

338.9

497.9

309.5

Community

Total

1736.2

565.9

2058.3

704.6

1946.5

674.0

Factor 1

596.1

291.4

937.2

412.6

815.6

406.9

Factor 2

731.7

152.3

676.4

177.1

679.1

170.8

Factor 3

278.5

161.2

301.8

208.1

295.1

193.3

Reliability

The SAS showed strong internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alphas of .96 in the undergraduate sample and .95 in the community sample. The scale scores were equally strong, with alphas ranging from .87 to .95. Test-retest reliability was examined in a subset of the undergraduate sample (n = 42), and suggested good stability of scores over time (r = .87, p < .01).

Validity

In order to establish discriminant validity, measures of mood (Depression, Anxiety, Stress Scale; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) and personality (International Personality Item Pool; Goldberg, 1999) were administered to both samples. SAS total scores were not simply a reflection of mood, showing only a very small correlation with anxiety, and were not a reflection of neuroticism or other personality traits. See Table 4 for additional details.

In order to establish construct validity, the SAS was administered along with measures of various aspects of sexuality, with overlapping and different measures within each sample. In the community sample, the SAS had a high correlation with the SOS. As well, lower SAS scores (i.e., greater erotophilia) were significantly Pearson Correlations With SAS Undergrad Sample Community Sample‌

& Byers, 1992), and more positive attitudes towards sex education of both male and female children (measure developed by the authors). Regression analyses indicated that the SAS was a better predictor of sexual functioning than was the SOS, particularly the Sexual Communication scale; otherwise, the two measures were equivalent in their prediction of sexual behaviour and attitudes (Purdon & Gordon, 2005).

In the undergraduate sample, lower SAS scores were significantly correlated with greater sexual satisfaction (Global Measure of Sexual Satisfaction; Lawrance & Byers, 1995), better sexual functioning (Golombok-Rust Inventory of Sexual Satisfaction; Rust & Golombok, 1998), higher scores on a measure of knowledge about sexual issues (e.g., anatomy, contraception, pregnancy, sexually transmitted infections [STIs], reproduction; developed by the authors), and more frequent use of birth control and STI protection (measure developed by the authors). The correlation between the SAS and antigay prejudice (Heterosexual Attitudes Toward Homosexuality Scale; Larsen, 1998) was not significant. However, the distribution of this measure was heavily skewed with the vast majority of the sample reporting little or no antigay prejudice, so there was little variance. See Table 5 for additional details.

Some group differences emerged. In both samples, males had lower SAS scores than females, t(370) = −5.16, p < .01 and t(185) = −3.19, p < .01 for the undergraduate and community samples respectively. Participants not currently practicing a religion had significantly lower SAS scores than those currently practicing a religion, t(164) = 2.23, p < .05. However, SAS scores did not differ according to sexual orientation.

For each item presented below, you are asked to rate how much discomfort you would experience using the following scale: How much discomfort would you feel in each situation? (Place this rating under column “D”)

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

Extremely Neutral Extremely Pleasurable Discomforting

D

  1. Wearing clothes that show off my sexually attractive features c
  2. Seeing two people kissing or fondling each other a
  3. Watching a movie scene from a major box office movie in which people were naked a
  4. Talking with my friends about my sex life
  5. Masturbating a
  6. Looking at hardcore or pornographic photos in a magazine (explicit scenes of the genitals and penetration)a
  7. Using sex toys, such as a vibrator, during sex with my partner a
  8. Exploring the erogenous, or sexually exciting, parts of my partner’s body c
  9. Hearing about someone engaging in a consensual sexual act that I personally would never want to engage in a
  10. Discussing my sexual fantasies with my partner c
  11. Having arousing sexual thoughts that are unrelated to my current sexual partner a
  12. Hearing about a woman who enjoyed sex and was sexually adventurous a
  13. Watching a “hardcore” or “pornographic” film a
  14. Being exposed to information about sexually transmitted infections b
  15. Kissing or fondling my partner in a public place
  16. Vocalizing my pleasure during sex with my partner c
  17. Watching a movie scene from a major box office movie in which people were kissing or fondling each other c
  18. Hearing about someone who has a biological sexual abnormality, such as undescended testicles, or a fertility problem b
  19. Reading books with sexually explicit passages c
  20. Agreeing to try sexual activities or positions that I find unusual but my partner suggests c
  21. Using sex toys, such as a vibrator, when I am alone a
  22. Engaging in foreplay with my partner c
  23. Finding myself becoming sexually aroused in response to something I never would have expected myself to be aroused by c
  24. Visiting Internet sites that feature erotic or softcore photos or video clips a
  25. Having arousing sexual thoughts that are related to my current sexual partner c
  26. Talking with my partner about his/her sexual fantasies c
  27. Talking with my friends about general matters of a sexual nature, such as menstruation, pregnancy, childbirth b
  28. Changing my clothes in a public change room that does not have privacy cubicles b
  29. Being exposed to information about contraceptive devices that require intimate genital contact (e.g., diaphragm, sponge, foam)b
  30. Overhearing other people (not parents) having sex a
  31. Watching a scene from a major box office movie in which people were engaging in sex a
  32. Exploring erogenous, or sexually exciting, parts of my body when I am alone a
  33. Someone knowing that I look at/watch erotic photos/films a
  34. Suggesting new sexual activities or positions to my partner c
  35. Visiting Internet sites that features hardcore or pornographic photos or video clips a
  36. Engaging in a casual sexual encounter (e.g., a one-night stand)a
  37. Being invited by an acquaintance/friend/partner to engage in an unusual sexual acta
  38. Hearing about sexual issues or matters from the newspaper or TV b
  39. Fantasizing about arousing sexual acts during sex with my partner in order to enhance my sexual excitement c
  40. Disclosing to my friends that I have a sexual problem b
  41. Answering questions about sexual matters such as conception b
  42. Someone overhearing me and my partner having sex a
  43. Being around others who are changing their clothes‌
  44. Being exposed to information about diseases of the sex organs, such as cervical cancer, testicular cancer, prostate cancer, breast cancer b‌
  45. Watching an “erotic” or “softporn” film (no explicit scenes of the genitals or penetration)a
  46. Allowing my partner to explore my erogenous, or sexually exciting, parts of my body c
  47. Someone knowing that I look at/watch pornographic photos/films a
  48. Changing activities or positions during sex with a partner to help ensure that I have an orgasm c
  49. Looking at erotic or softcore photos in a magazine a
  50. Telling my partner what pleases me and does not please me sexually c
  51. Hearing about people I don’t consider to be sexual engaging in sex, such as the elderly, my parents, disabled people b
  52. Having a conversation with my friends about their sex lives b
  53. Fantasizing about arousing sexual thoughts during masturbation in order to enhance my sexual excitement a
  54. Watching coverage of the Gay Pride Day parade b
  55. Being exposed to information about contraceptives and contraceptive use b
  56. Completing questionnaires about my sexuality b

a = Solitary and Impersonal Sexual Expression (Factor 1)

b = Exposure to Information (Factor 2)

c = Sexual Communication (Factor 3)

References

Fisher, W. A., Byrne, D., White, L. A., & Kelley, K. (1988). Erotophobiaerotophilia as a dimension of personality. The Journal of Sex Research, 25, 123–151.

Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-fac- tor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe, 7 (pp. 7–28). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.

Larsen, K. S. (1998). Heterosexual attitudes toward homosexuality scale. In C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber, R. Bauserman, G. Schreer, & S. L. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of sexuality-related measures (pp. 394–395). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Lawrance, K., & Byers, E. S. (1992). Sexual satisfaction: A social exchange perspective. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, Quebec.

Lawrance, K., & Byers, E. S. (1995). Sexual satisfaction in long-term heterosexual relationships: The Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction. Personal Relationships, 2, 267–285.

Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour, Research and Therapy, 33, 335–343.

Purdon, C., & Gordon, C. (2005, November). Development of the Sexual Anxiety Scale. Poster presented at the Association for the Advancement of Behavior and Cognitive Therapies Annual Meeting, Washington, DC.

Rust, J., & Golombok, S. (1998). The GRISS: A psychometric scale and profile of sexual dysfunction. In C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber,

R. Bauserman, G. Schreer, & S. L. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of sexuality-related measures (pp. 192–194). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

x