Sexual Opinion scale

Sexual Opinion scale

B. J. RYE,St. Jerome’s University at the University of Waterloo

GLENN J. MEANEYWilfrid Laurier University

WILLIAM A. FISHERUniversity of Western Ontario

The personality dimension of erotophobia-erotophilia is conceptualized as a learned disposition to respond to sexual stimuli with negative-to-positive affect and evaluation and is believed to determine avoidance or approach responses to sexual stimuli. The Sexual Opinion Survey (SOS) is a measure of this trait (Fisher, Byrne, White, & Kelley, 1988).

Description

The SOS (see Exhibit 1) consists of 21 items intended to assess affective and evaluative responses to sexual stimuli (autosexual, homosexual, heterosexual behavior; sexual fantasy; visual sexual stimuli). Each item depicts a sexual situation and an affective response to it; individuals are asked to indicate degree of agreement or disagreement with the statement (scale development is described in Fisher, 1998, and Fisher et al., 1988). Terminology in some items was updated by Fisher (1998) because some of the original wording was no longer in use or had changed meaning. Translations of the SOS into Hebrew, French, Finnish, German, Portuguese, and Japanese are available; recently, it has also been translated into Spanish (Sierra, Ortega, & Zubeidat, 2006) and Dutch (Dodge, Sandfort, Yarber, & de Wit, 2005).

A short form of the SOS was developed by Semph (1979; see Exhibit 2) using five items established as strong predictors of full scale scores (e.g., Table 2, Sample 1: = .84). A scale conceptually based on the SOS was designed for use with very young adolescent girls (Rye et al., 2008; see Exhibit 3) and highly correlated with the SOS in a sample of first-year university students (= .74, = 55).

Extensive construct validity for the SOS was reported by Fisher et al. (1988) and Fisher (1998); therefore, we will focus on literature published since 1998. The SOS has been used extensively with North American university students and has been used to assess erotophobia-erotophilia in university students in Australia (Beatson & Halloran, 2007), Israel (Birnbaum & Gillath, 2006), and the Netherlands (Dodge et al., 2005), as well as nonstudent populations including adult women (Graham, Sanders, & Milhausen,

2006), high-school men (Sierra et al., 2006), incarcerated men (Garland, Morgan, & Beer, 2005), women who have had an abortion and nonpregnant women at health clinics (Bradshaw & Slade, 2005), and users and nonusers of erotica (Lawyer, 2008). Normative data from these studies, when available, are summarized in Table 1; the current authors’ normative data are summarized in Table 2.

Response Mode and Timing

Completion of the SOS usually takes 10 minutes or less. General instructions are provided (see Exhibit 1); additional instructions may be added for participants unfamiliar with Likert-type responding.

Scoring

Fisher (1998) specified the following scoring scheme: First, score responses from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree); second, add scores from Items 2, 5, 6, 12, 13, 14, 15,

16, 19, and 20; third, subtract the sum of Items 1, 3, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 17, 18, and 21; fourth, add 67. This scheme results in a number between 0 (erotophobic) and 126 (erotophilic); missing data can be handled for positively and negatively worded items by substituting the mean score for the other

TABLE 1

Normative Data From Published Research

Study

Subsamples

N

M

SD

Group Differencesa

Scoringa

High Score

Notes

Barak et al. (1999)

n/a

31

73.36

17.21

n/a

Fisher

Erotophilia

Undergraduate

men

Bogaert & Sadava

n/a

792

83.97

22.16

n/a

22 to 137

Erotophilia

Adults

(2002)

Bradshaw & Slade

Abortion

98

76

19

Significant

Fisher

Erotophilia

Women from

(2005)

No abortion

51

69

21

difference

health clinics

Dodge et al. (2005)

United States

133

3.07

1.03

Significant

Average

Erotophobia

Undergraduate

Netherlands

109

2.61

1.00

difference

men

Forbes et al. (2003)

n/a

211

2.68

.70

No gender

1 to 6

Erotophilia

University

difference

Average

students; short

version

Garcia (1999)

Men

77

5.7 ± 19.1

n/a

No significant

Fisher –67

Erotophilia

Introductory

Women

84

−6.7 ± 19.3

n/a

difference

−67 to +59

psychology

students

Geer & Robertson

Men

115

66.72

23.89

Significant

Not described

Erotophilia

Psychology

(2005)

Women

137

59.56

19.32

difference

undergraduates

Hogben et al. (2001)

Men

177

59.88

11.86

Significant

Items:

Erotophilia

Introductory

Women

176

71.15

15.96

difference

1 to 6

psychology

students

Lawyer (2008)

Erotica users

34

93.3

22.7

No significant

Not described

Erotophilia

Men and

Erotica

37

84.9

22.1

difference

women

non-users

Sanders et al. (2006)

Condom

102

3.9

1.2

Significant

Average

Erotophobia

Sexually active

appliers

difference

women

Condom

220

4.2

1.1

non-appliers

Shim et al. (2007) n/a 151 76 19 n/a Fisher Erotophilia Introductory communications students

a Scoring information based on published literature or personal communication with authors. “Fisher” indicates that scores were calculated as specified by Fisher (1998) and in the Scoring section. “Average” indicates that negatively worded items were reverse coded and then all items were averaged. All items were scored on a 7-point Likert-type scale except where noted.

TABLE 2‌

Normative Data From the Authors’ Research

Sample

Subsamples

N

M

SD

Group Differences

Notes

1

Men

854

3.83

.50

Significant difference

Introductory human sexuality

Women

2007

3.60

.57

students; 23 classes enrolled

Heterosexuala

2657

3.63

.55

Significant difference

between 1999 and 2007

Queera

207

4.07

.52

2

Men

27

4.37

.83

No significant

Introductory psychology

Women

39

4.05

.91

difference

students agreed to participate in sex study

3

n/a

135

4.14

1.15

n/a

Introductory psychology students department mass-testing

4

Men

43

4.92

1.18

Significant

Homophobia awareness

Women

70

4.31

1.11

difference

workshop (Rye & Meaney, 2009); short version without Item 21

5

Heterosexual

49

4.39

.67

Lesbians more

Adult women

AISb

44

4.59

.76

erotophilic than

Lesbian

45

4.85

.63

heterosexuals

Note. Except where noted, all samples completed the full, 21-item SOS. Scoring: Individual items were rated on a 1 to 7 scale, except for Sample 1 (1 to 5). Negatively worded items were reverse scored. Then, all items were averaged. Higher scores indicate erotophilia. Missing data were ignored provided completion of half the items.

a5-point self-identification scale: 1–2 = heterosexual, 3–5 = queer (bisexual or homosexual) bAIS = Androgen Insensitivity Syndrome

positively or negatively worded items, respectively (stipu- lating completion of half the relevant items). In practice, some researchers simply add the items after reverse coding negatively worded items (resulting in a score from 21 to 147), or average across all items (with negatively worded items reverse coded, resulting in a score from 1 to 7). There are advantages to each scoring scheme. Averaging the items, for example, results in a score on the same scale as the individual items (i.e., 1 to 7) and minimizes the impact of missing data (i.e., a few missing items can be omitted). However, researchers conducting cross- study comparisons should be aware that scores from different studies may not be directly comparable. Scoring schemes should not affect relationships between the SOS and other variables.

Reliability

Since Fisher (1998) reported on the reliability of the SOS, high internal consistency has been found among various samples of university students (Cronbach’s α = .76 to .89; see References) and using a Hebrew version of the SOS (α = .85; Birnbaum & Gillath, 2006); internal consistency is about equal for men and women (e.g., Smith & Nave, 2007). Lower internal consistency (α = .63) was found for the short version (Beatson & Halloran, 2007). A scale conceptually based on the SOS designed for very young adolescent girls had similar internal consistency (α = .63; Rye et al., 2008). Substantial test-retest reliability of the full scale was found with a sample of university students over an interval of several weeks (Table 2, Sample 2: = .61, = 35). Although the SOS is highly internally consistent, some researchers have attempted to identify subscales. Sierra et al. (2006), using a Spanish sample, reported four factors (erotophilia, erotophobia, homophobia, and unconventional sex); a similar factor structure was reported using a Norwegian sample (Rise, Traeen, & Kraft, 1993). Based on our analysis (Table 2, Sample 1), we recommend a two- factor solution when investigation of factors is desired: (a) open sexual display and sexual variety, and (b) homoeroticism—a refinement of the three-factor solution suggested by Gilbert & Gamache, 1984). Further investigation is required to demonstrate the generality and stability of sets of factors and the utility of factor-based subscales.

Validity

Since publication in the previous edition of the Handbook of Sexuality-Related Measures (Fisher, 1998), the con- struct validity of the SOS continues to be well established through research with theoretically relevant variables. In particular, SOS scores are predictive of prior sexual media exposure (Bogaert, 2001), subjective sexual arousal (Nobre et al., 2004), sexual arousal and anxiety (and, indirectly, sexual self-schema; Cyranowski & Andersen, 1998), self- reported sexual behavior (i.e., masturbation, multiple partners, unprotected sex; Durant, Carey, & Schroder, 2002), implicit sexual attitudes (Geer & Robertson, 2005), perceptions of consumer items as a means of attracting sex- ual partners or enhancing sex (Gould, 1995), sexual exci- tation and inhibition (Graham et al., 2006; Janssen, Vorst, Finn, & Bancroft, 2002a, 2002b; Wilson, Holm, Bishop, & Borowiak, 2002), homonegativity (Mahaffey, Bryan, & Hutchison, 2005), condom application (i.e., women

who applied condoms on their male partners were more erotophilic than those who did not; Sanders et al., 2006), “short-term, unrestricted mating orientation” (Schmitt, Shackelford, Duntley, Tooke, & Buss, 2001), willingness to be exposed to unsolicited Internet pornography (Shim, Lee, & Paul, 2007), and sexual fantasy (Sierra et al., 2006). Garcia (1999) found that participants higher in erotophilia were more likely to rate themselves as sexually experienced, sexually deviant, holding liberal or permissive sexual attitudes, and being sexually responsive.

In our research (Table 2, Sample 1), SOS scores were weakly to moderately correlated with a variety of sexual attitudes (e.g., approval of premarital sexual intercourse, accessibility of birth control), sexual behaviors with others (e.g., number of sexual partners, having orally stimulated a partner’s genitals), and autosexual behavior (e.g., use of mechanical aids, frequency of masturbation, also reported by Zamboni & Crawford, 2002). The SOS did not predict self-reported sexual dysfunction. Erotophilia correlated moderately with being gay or lesbian, attitudes toward homosexuality, and having gay friends or acquaintances.

Although previous research has found erotophobiaerotophilia to be a consistent predictor of sexual health behaviors (see Fisher, 1998), little current research has investigated this connection. Our data (Table 2, Sample 1) suggest that the SOS was generally unrelated to sexual health behaviors, with a few exceptions—for example, erotophilia predicted self-reported likelihood of pap exams and breast self-examination (but not testicular self-examination). Wilkinson, Holahan, and Drane-Edmundson (2002) found that erotophilia predicted safer-sex behaviors for people with cooperative partners, but not for people with uncooperative partners.

Erotophobia-erotophilia has been found to correlate with authoritarianism (Bogaert, 2001; Hogben, Byrne, Hamburger, & Osland, 2001), sensation-seeking (Bogaert, 2001), and anxious (Bogaert & Sadava, 2002) and dismissive (Schmitt et al., 2001) attachment styles. Hogben et al. (2001) found that SOS scores were related to aggression (although Bogaert did not) and gender-typing for men (again, not found by Bogaert). Our data (Table 2, Sample 4) indicate that SOS scores moderately predict authoritarian- ism, religiosity, and knowledge of sexual anatomy.

Discriminant validity is provided by evidence that the SOS does not correlate with social desirability, aggression, dominance, intelligence, or psychoticism (Bogaert, 2001). Our own data (Table 2, Sample 4) suggest that erotophobiaerotophilia is uncorrelated with social desirability.

Other Information

Although intended to measure an affective disposition, the SOS is generally used as an indicator of general attitudes toward sexuality. Erotophobia-erotophilia tends to be used as one of several potential predictors of an outcome measure of interest, and is occasionally used as an outcome measure in itself.

Sexual Opinion Survey

Please respond to each item as honestly as you can. There are no right and wrong answers, and your answers will be completely anonymous.

  1. I think it would be very entertaining to look at hard-core erotica.

    I strongly agree I strongly disagree a

  2. Erotica is obviously filthy and people should not try to describe it as anything else.

  3. Swimming in the nude with a member of the opposite sex would be an exciting experience.

  4. Masturbation can be an exciting experience.

  5. If I found that a close friend of mine was a homosexual, it would annoy me.

  6. If people thought I was interested in oral sex, I would be embarrassed.

  7. Engaging in group sex is an entertaining idea.

  8. I personally find that thinking about engaging in sexual intercourse is arousing.

  9. Seeing an erotic movie would be sexually arousing to me.

  10. Thoughts that I may have homosexual tendencies would not worry me at all.

  11. The idea of my being physically attracted to members of the same sex is not depressing.

  12. Almost all sexually explicit material is nauseating.

  13. It would be emotionally upsetting to me to see someone exposing themselves publicly.

  14. Watching an exotic dancer of the opposite sex would not be very exciting.

  15. I would not enjoy seeing an erotic movie.

  16. When I think about seeing pictures showing someone of the same sex as myself masturbating, it nauseates me.

  17. The thought of engaging in unusual sex practices is highly arousing.

  18. Manipulating my genitals would probably be an arousing experience.

  19. I do not enjoy daydreaming about sexual matters.

  20. I am not curious about explicit erotica.

  21. The thought of having long-term sexual relations with more than one sex partner is not disgusting to me.

Note. Score the survey as follows: First, score responses from 1 = I strongly agree to 7 = I strongly disagree. Second, add scores from Items 2, 5, 6, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 19, and 20. Third, subtract from this total the sum of Items 1, 3, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 17, 18, and 21. Fourth, add 67 to this quantity. Scores range

from 0 (most erotophobic) to 126 (most erotophilic). For the short form, administer Items 12, 4, 13, 17, and 21 (in this order, renumbered 1–5). Score responses from 1 = I strongly agree to 7 = I strongly disagree.

To reflect current usage, the terms pornography and pornographic from the original scale have been replaced with erotic or sexually explicit material and

stripper has been replaced with exotic dancer.

a The scale is repeated after each item.

Sexual Opinion Survey—Short Form

  1. Almost all sexually explicit material is nauseating.

    I strongly agree I strongly disagree a

  2. Masturbation can be an exciting experience.

  3. It would be emotionally upsetting to me to see someone exposing themselves publicly.

  4. The thought of engaging in unusual sex practices is highly arousing.

  5. The thought of having long-term sexual relations with more than one sex partner is not disgusting to me.

Note. Score as follows: Score responses from 1 = I strongly agree to 7 = I strongly disagree; then reverse code items 1 and 3; then add all responses. Scores can range from 5 (most erotophilic) to 35 (most erotophobic). To reflect current usage, the terms pornography and pornographic from the original scale have been replaced with erotic or sexually explicit material.

a The scale is repeated after each item.


Comfort With Sexuality Matters for Very Young Adolescent Girls

Please respond to each item as honestly as you can. There are no right and wrong answers, and your answers will be completely anonymous.

  1. It is not OK for a person to have more than one sex partner during their lifetime.

    I strongly disagree I strongly agree a

  2. It is OK for a person to masturbate if it makes him/her feel good.

  3. It is OK for two men to have sex with each other or two women to have sex with each other.

  4. It is not OK for people to have sexual intercourse unless they are in a committed relationship.

  5. It is OK to enjoy being sexually aroused (turned on) by a sexy story, picture, or movie.

  6. Oral sexb is disgusting to me.

Note. Score as follows: First, score responses from 1 (I strongly disagree) to 7 (I strongly agree); second, reverse code Items 1, 4, and 6; third, average all 6 items. Scores range from 1 (least comfortable) to 7 (most comfortable). Missing data can be ignored provided participants complete at least half of the items.

a The scale is repeated after each item.

b Our initial sample of Grade 7 girls generally did not know what “oral sex” was. The term was defined briefly by the project manager.

 

Address correspondence to B. J. Rye, Department of Psychology, St. Jerome’s University, 290 Westmount Road North, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G3; e-mail: [email protected]

References

Barak, A., Fisher, W. A., Belfry, S., & LaShambe, D. R. (1999). Sex, guys, and cyberspace: Effects of Internet pornography and individual differences on men’s attitudes toward women. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 11(1), 63–91.

Beatson, R. M., & Halloran, M. J. (2007). Humans rule! The effects of creatureliness reminders, mortality salience and self-esteem on attitudes towards animals. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 619–632.

Birnbaum, G. E., & Gillath, O. (2006). Measuring subgoals of the sexual behavioral system: What is sex good for? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 675–701.

Bogaert, A. F. (2001). Personality, individual differences, and preferences for the sexual media. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 30, 29–53.

Bogaert, A. F., & Sadava, S. (2002). Adult attachment and sexual behavior. Personal Relationships, 9, 191–204.

Bradshaw, Z., & Slade, P. (2005). The relationships between induced abortion, attitudes towards sexuality and sexual problems. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 20, 391–406.

Cyranowski, J. M., & Andersen, B. L. (1998). Schemas, sexuality, and romantic attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1364–1379.

Dodge, B., Sandfort, T. G. M., Yarber, W. L., & de Wit, J. (2005). Sexual health among male college students in the United States and the Netherlands. American Journal of Health Behavior, 29, 172–182.

Durant, L. E., Carey, M. P., & Schroder, K. E. E. (2002). Effects of ano- nymity, gender, and erotophilia on the quality of data obtained from self-reports of socially sensitive behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 439–467.

Fisher, W. A. (1998). The Sexual Opinion Survey. In C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber, R. Bauserman, G. Schreer, & S. L. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of sexuality-related measures (pp. 218–221). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Fisher, W. A., Byrne, D., White, L. A., & Kelley, K. (1988). Erotophobia- erotophilia as a dimension of personality. The Journal of Sex Research, 25, 123–151.

Forbes, G. B., Adams-Curtis, L. E., Hamm, N. R., & White, K. B. (2003). Perceptions of the woman who breastfeeds: The role of erotophobia, sexism, and attitudinal variables. Sex Roles, 49, 379–388.

Garcia, L. T. (1999). The certainty of the sexual self-concept. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 8, 263–270.

Garland, J. T., Morgan, R. D., & Beer, A. M. (2005). Impact of time in prison and security level on inmates’ sexual attitude, behavior, and identity. Psychological Services, 2, 151–162.

Geer, J. H., & Robertson, G. G. (2005). Implicit attitudes in sexuality: Gender differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 671–677.

Gilbert, F. S., & Gamache, M. P. (1984). The Sexual Opinion Survey: Structure and use. The Journal of Sex Research, 20, 293–309.

Gould, S. J. (1995). Sexualized aspects of consumer behavior: An empiri- cal investigation of consumer lovemaps. Psychology and Marketing, 12, 395–413.

Graham, C. A., Sanders, S. A., & Milhausen, R. R. (2006). The Sexual Excitation/Sexual Inhibition Inventory for Women: Psychometric properties. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 397–409.

Hogben, M., Byrne, D., Hamburger, M. E., & Osland, J. (2001). Legitimized aggression and sexual coercion: Individual differences in cultural spillover. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 26–43.

Janssen, E., Vorst, H., Finn, P., & Bancroft, J. (2002a). The Sexual Inhibition (SIS) and Sexual Excitation (SES) Scales: I. Measuring sexual inhibition and excitation proneness in men. Journal of Sex Research, 39, 114–126.

Janssen, E., Vorst, H., Finn, P., & Bancroft, J. (2002b). The Sexual Inhibition (SIS) and Sexual Excitation (SES) Scales: II. Predicting psychophysiological response patterns. The Journal of Sex Research, 39, 127–132.

Lawyer, S. R. (2008). Probability and delay discounting of erotic stimuli. Behavioural Processes, 79, 36–42.

Mahaffey, A. L., Bryan, A., & Hutchison, K. E. (2005). Using startle eye blink to measure the affective component of antigay bias. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 37–45.

Nobre, P. J., Wiegel, M., Bach, A. K., Weisberg, R. B., Brown, T. A., Wincze, J. P., et al. (2004). Determinants of sexual arousal and the accuracy of its self-estimation in sexually functional males. The Journal of Sex Research, 41, 363–371.

Rise, J., Traeen, B., & Kraft, P. (1993). The Sexual Opinion Survey Scale: A study on dimensionality in Norwegian adolescents. Health Education Research, 8, 485–492.

Rye, B. J. & Meaney, G. J. (2009). Impact of a homonegativity aware- ness workshop on attitudes toward homosexuality. Journal of Homosexuality, 56(1), 31–55.

Rye, B. J., Yessis, J., Brunk, T., McKay, A., Morris, S., & Meaney, G. J. (2008). Outcome evaluation of Girl Time: Grade 7/8 Healthy Sexuality ProgramCanadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 17, 15–36.

Sanders, S. A., Graham, C. A., Yarber, W. L., Crosby, R. A., Dodge, B., & Milhausen, R. R. (2006). Women who put condoms on male partners: Correlates of condom application. American Journal of Health Behavior, 30, 460–466.

Schmitt, D. P., Shackelford, T. K., Duntley, J., Tooke, W., & Buss, D. M. (2001). The desire for sexual variety as a key to understanding basic human mating strategies. Personal Relationships. Special Issue: Evolutionary Approaches to Relationships, 8, 425–455.

Semph, M. E. (1979). Emotional orientation towards sexuality: Its relation to expecting and perceiving contraceptive side effects. Unpublished honors thesis. University of Western Ontario, Department of Psychology, London, Ontario, Canada.

Shim, J. W., Lee, S., & Paul, B. (2007). Who responds to unsolicited sexually explicit materials on the Internet? The role of individual differ- ences. CyberPsychology and Behavior, 10, 71–79.

Sierra, J. C., Ortega, V., & Zubeidat, I. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis of a Spanish version of the sex fantasy questionnaire: Assessing gen- der differences. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 32, 137–159.

Smith, G. E., & Nave, C. S. (2007). Illness transmission mode and perceiver personality: Factors affecting stigmatized perceptions of patients and sexual illness. Social Behavior and Personality, 35, 853–860.

Wilkinson, A.V., Holahan, C. J., & Drane-Edmundson, E. W. (2002). Predicting safer sex practices: The interactive role of partner coopera- tion and cognitive factors. Psychology and Health, 17, 697–709.

Wilson, B. A., Holm, J. E., Bishop, K. L., & Borowiak, D. M. (2002). Predicting responses to sexually aggressive stories: The role of con- sent, interest in sexual aggression, and overall sexual interest. The Journal of Sex Research, 39, 275–283.

Zamboni, B. D., & Crawford, I. (2002). Using masturbation in sex therapy: Relationships between masturbation, sexual desire, and sexual fan- tasy. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 14(2/3), 123–141.

x