
Instructions:
Below is a list of statements dealing with your general feelings about yourself. There are four possible answers for each of the 10 questions, from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree.
Tap the box to indicate how strongly you agree or disagree with each statement.
|
Strongly Agree |
Agree |
Disagree |
Strongly Disagree |
|
On the whole, I am satisfied with myself |
3 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
At times, I think I am no good at all |
0 |
1 |
2 |
3 |
I feel that I have a number of good qualities |
3 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
I am able to do things as well as most other people |
3 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
I feel I do not have much to be proud of |
0 |
1 |
2 |
3 |
I certainly feel useless at times |
0 |
1 |
2 |
3 |
I feel that I’m a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others |
3 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
I wish I could have more respect for myself |
0 |
1 |
2 |
3 |
All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure |
0 |
1 |
2 |
3 |
I take a positive attitude toward myself |
3 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
Description
The RSES measures self-esteem using ten items answered on a four-point Likert-type scale — from strongly agree to strongly disagree. The RSES is one of the most widely used measures of self-esteem (Sinclair et al., 2010). Self-esteem is not a unitary construct and has been divided equally to measure two 5-item facets; self-competence and self-liking. – Self-competence (SC) is understood as one’s instrumental value. Schmitt and Allik (2005) defined self-competence as ”feeling you are confident, capable and efficacious”(p. 625). – Self-liking (SL) is defined as one’s sense of intrinsic self value (Sinclair et al., 2010). Schmitt and Allik (2005) defined self-liking as ”feeling you are good and socially relevant” (p. 625). A central assumption in psychology has been that high self-esteem is beneficial and low self-esteem is detrimental (Crocker & Park, 2004). However, a more nuanced view of self-esteem is emerging, suggesting the benefits of high trait self-esteem are restricted to enhanced initiative and happiness (Baumiester, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003). Crocker and Park (2004) point out that self-esteem is associated with self-focus interpersonal and achievement goals, and high self-esteem can produce costs in terms of loss of relatedness, increased competitiveness and lower concern for group wellbeing. These researchers argue psychologists should be less concerned in fostering the amount of self-esteem and more concerned with the processes by which individuals pursue it.
The scale has good predictive validity, as well as internal consistency and test–retest reliability (Schmitt & Allik, 2005; Torrey, Mueser, McHugo, & Drake, 2000). Cronbach coefficient has been shown to be high (M = 0.81) supporting the internal coherence of the scale. Sinclair et al. (2010) suggest that self-esteem scores are highly dependent on temporal affect, and therefore the scale may not capture trait based self-esteem adequately. Despite this, test-retest reliability over a period of 2 weeks reveals correlations of .85 and .88, indicating excellent stability. The RSES demonstrates a Guttman scale coefficient of reproducibility of .92, indicating excellent internal consistency. Interpretation
The minimum total score is 0 and the maximum is 30, with higher scores representing higher self-esteem. Results consist of four raw scores and four percentiles: Total self-esteem, self-competence (sum of first five items), self-liking (sum of second five items) and self-competence minus self liking (SC-SL). Percentiles indicate how the client’s self-esteem compares to Sinclair’s adult sample. Sinclair et al. (2010) found that among a sample of 503 adults (M = 44.7 years, SD = 16.3) the average self-esteem score was 22.62 (SD = 5.8). The self-competence subscale had a mean of 12.01 (SD=2.82). The mean for self-liking was 10.62 (SD = 3.35). Additionally, when the raw self-liking score was taken away from the raw self-competence score (SC-SL), the average difference was 1.39 (SD=2.15). Despite higher self-esteem not always being more adaptive, evidence shows that self-esteem is negatively and linearly related to disorders of mood and anxiety (Greenberg et al., 1992; Lightsey et al., 2006; Neustadt et al., 2006; Torrey et al., 2000), so higher self-esteem has been shown to be protective against some mental disorders. Developer
Number Of Questions
10
References
Sinclair SJ; Blais MA; Gansler DA; Sandberg E; Bistis K; LoCicero A. Psychometric properties of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: overall and across demographic groups living within the United States. Eval Health Prof;33(1):56-80, 2010
Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem. Psychological Bulletin; Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 392.
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological science in the public interest, 4(1), 1-44.
Developer Reference:
This content is licensed under a CC-BY license. The CC-BY licenses grant rights of use the scales in your studies (the measurement instrument and its documentation), but do not replace copyright. This remains with the copyright holder, and you have to cite us as the source.
Mohammed Looti, PSYCHOLOGICAL SCALES (2023) Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES). Retrieved from https://scales.arabpsychology.com/s/rosenberg-self-esteem-scale-rses-2/. DOI: 10.13140/RG.2.2.31575.96163